الملاحظات
الفصل الأول: مقدمة: الجوانب المتعدِّدة لعلم النفس والأوجُه الكثيرة للأفلام
(1)
Keyser (1992).
(2)
Diamond, Wrye and Sabbadini (2007) point out that when
Freud published his first significant work, Studies in Hysteria in 1895 (co-authored with Josef
Breuer), the Lumiere brothers were screening what is widely considered
to be the first nonfiction film, Workers Leaving
the Lumiere Factory. The scientific-minded American
Psychological Association had been founded a few years earlier in 1892
(Wertheimer, 1987).
(3)
Freud’s appearance at Clark left an aura that pervaded even
the physical space. Many of us were convinced that the reason the
university never remodeled the worn wooden staircase was because Freud
had made the stairs sacred by setting foot on
them.
(4)
Werner (1980).
(5)
Many of the professors at Clark at that time had been
mentored by Werner, including Bernard Kaplan, Leonard Cirillo, Roger
Bibace, Seymour Wapner, Robert Baker, and the neuropsychologist Edith
Kaplan. Other Clark professors who influenced my thinking were
socioculturalists James Wertsch and James Gee and narrativists Michael
Bamberg and Nancy Budwig.
(6)
Kristen and Dine Young (2009).
(7)
Amateur short films are becoming much more
available, thanks to digital cameras and YouTube. Even
as I write, my children are collaborating with
neighborhood kids to make their own movie. Perhaps in
another decade someone will write a book on The Psychology of
YouTube.
(8)
Wade and Tavris (2005) define psychology as “the discipline
concerned with behavior and mental processes and how they are affected
by an organism’s physical state, mental state and external environment”
(p. 3).
(9)
Method has strong religious connotations, deriving from the
Greek root methodos, meaning “the
way.” The derivation calls to mind Jesus’ proclamation, “I am the way,
the truth and the life” (John 14:6). Psychologists have been known to be
almost as serious about their
methods.
(10)
Interestingly, the only book on psychology and the movies
as freeranging as Gladwell’s approach is Munsterberg’s 1916 work which
mixes history, technology, experimental psychology, textual
interpretation, aesthetic philosophy and imaginative
speculation.
(11)
Sternberg and Grigorenko (2001).
(12)
Another effective way of making this point is John Saxe’s
poem The Blind Men and the Elephant
in which several blind men investigate an isolated body part (tusk,
trunk, ear, etc.) of an elephant and come to erroneous conclusions about
the nature of an elephant (concluding it is a spear, snake, fan, etc.).
This poem is used by Tavris and Wade in their novel textbook, Psychology in Perspective, which introduces
the field of psychology in a more cohesive
manner.
(13)
The symbolic framework presented here is a simplification
of the model presented by Werner and Kaplan (1984) in Symbol Formation. They draw their
perspective, in part, from the symbolic philosophy of Ernst Cassirer
(1955–1957) and the rhetorical method of Kenneth Burke (1973).
Symbolization, as understood by Werner and Kaplan, is as amenable to
literary interpretation as it is to
experimentation.
(14)
Since most of the examples used in this book refer to how
visual and linguistic symbols are embedded in stories, narrative theory (i.e., theories about how
stories are constructed and how they are received by listeners/viewers)
pops up with some regularity. This theme is central in Chapter 9 in
which parallels are made between stories in movies and stories in
identity construction (McAdams, 1993).
(15)
The fact that symbols have more
than one level of meaning is taken by numerous writers,
including Carl Jung (1964) and Paul Ricoeur (1970) in his study of
Freud, as the defining aspect of symbolization.
(16)
Even if a small, independent film reaches “only” a few
thousand viewers, it is still a significant social event, especially if
a passionate “cult” audience becomes strongly attached to
it.
(17)
These symbolic events are psychological both because interpretations comment on
human nature (e.g., how people displace unacceptable tendencies like
aggression on to acceptable actions such as heroism) and because the
transformation between a symbolic object and its meaning requires
thought (i.e., mental activity or cognitive
processing).
الفصل الثاني: البحث عن معنًى: التفسيرات السيكولوجية في الأفلام
(1)
Greenberg (1975).
(2)
Payne (1989b).
(3)
Hopcke (1989).
(4)
Indick (2004).
(5)
Murphy (1996).
(6)
The open-ended use of “text” as an underlying narrative runs
counter to the everyday definition of text as something written (as in a
“textbook”), but going with the convention in literary, film and rhetorical
studies, I sometimes refer to films as “texts.”
(7)
Kracauer (1960) and Bazin (1967) are commonly associated with
championing film as an exercise in realism; this attitude found expression
in Italian neo-realism (Rome, Open City)
and cinema verité (Don’t Look Back).
Andrew (1976) asserts that realism ran counter to the earliest trends in
film criticism that highlighted films for their dreamlike qualities
exemplified by German expressionism (The Cabinet of
Dr. Caligari) and surrealism (Un
Chien Andalou). Many modern studies of film in both
psychology (Packer, 2007) and philosophy (McGinn, 2005) continue to
emphasize film as dream.
(8)
Bordwell (1989a) steps outside particular theoretical
frameworks to cogently explicate the general process that all interpreters
use to make meaning out of films.
(9)
The most prominent interpretive approaches in the early history
of film are reviewed by Andrew (1976). Casetti (1999) continues the task of
reviewing film theory through 1995.
(10)
There are many possibilities for types of behavior (e.g.,
farming, flying airplanes) and types of people (e.g., private
detectives, butlers) that could receive attention from social scientists
but have not. Psychotherapy and mental illness, however, have been the
topics of so much special interest, I consider them in detail in Chapter
3.
(11)
How movies impact the attitudes of audiences is considered
in more detail in Chapter 8.
(12)
Rendleman (2008).
(13)
See Krippendorff (2003) for an overview of content analysis
methods.
(14)
The interpretations of clever critics are discussed later
in this chapter. Such critics are typically unimpressed by content
analyses since content analytic categories have
to be stated in a way that everybody can understand. The joke goes that they need
to be so obvious, they could be identified by trained monkeys (or
graduate students, whichever are available). The virtues of
critics—cleverness, subtlety, and originality—may become liabilities
when it comes to content analyses.
(15)
It is not an accident that these topics correspond to the
social concerns of recent decades. Despite occasional claims of
neutrality, the social sciences do indeed swim in the cultural stream,
either as reflections or agents of change. The topics that receive
attention in this chapter have also been studied for their effects on audience members considered in
Chapter 8.
(16)
Wilson et al.
(2002).
(17)
Mean Girls is partly
based on Queen Bees and Wannabes,
Rosalind Wiseman’s nonfiction self-help book about adolescent female
cliques, which itself draws on research on relational aggression by
developmental psychologists such as Nicki Crick
(2002).
(18)
Coyne and Whitehead (2008).
(19)
Greenberg (1994).
(20)
See Gunter (2002) for review of research on sexual content
in media.
(21)
Cowan et al.
(1988).
(22)
An original content analysis by Molitor and Sapolsky (1993)
was followed by a critique by Linz and Donnerstein (1994) and then a
rebuttal by Molitor and Sapolsky (1994).
(23)
Welsh (2010).
(24)
See Sarafino (2008) for summary.
(25)
Glantz and Kacirk (2004).
(26)
Hazan, Lipton, and Glantz (1994).
(27)
Ricoeur (1974: pp. 12-13).
(28)
Ricoeur (1974: p. 99).
(29)
Examples of the numerous book-length studies that use
traditional psychodynamic theory to analyze film include Greenberg
(1975; 1993) and Indick (2004); many other interpretations have appeared
in journals like Psychoanalytic
Review and The International
Journal of Psychoanalysis. In addition, semiotic and
post-modern variations of Freudian theory are discussed in the
“Spectatorship” section of this chapter.
(30)
There are periodic attempts to declare Freudian theory
dead. Literary critic Frederick Crews’ (1995) sharp dismissal of the
scientific validity of psychoanalysis was at the center of a 1990s
debate known as the Freud Wars (see Forrester, 1998 for a defense of
Freud). Despite such battles, Freudian theory continues to thrive in the
humanities, and modern variations of psychoanalysis remain a powerful
force in mental health treatment, with some psychologists and
psychiatrists arguing that crucial elements of Freud’s approach are
validated by both research on effective psychotherapy (Shedler, 2010)
and modern neuroscience (Schore, 2003).
(31)
See Hall’s A Primer of Freudian
Psychology (1999) as a classic summary of Freudian
theory.
(32)
Freud (1960b: p. 58).
(33)
Psychodynamic is a broad term that covers Freud’s original
psychoanalysis and the many spin-off theories that came
later.
(34)
Greenberg (1975).
(35)
Greenberg (1975: p. 14).
(36)
Cocks (1991).
(37)
When it comes to Stanley Kubrick, one can never be sure
what was intended. One of my professors warned us never to underestimate
Kubrick’s attention to detail since the only thing more obsessively
conceived than one of his films was big-time
advertising.
(38)
Modern cognitive science has a mixed view of the
psychoanalytic contention that people unconsciously perceive every
aspect of their environment. On one hand, there is evidence that the
mind is highly selective about what information it processes and
remembers. At the same time, people do process and react to certain
environmental stimuli which they cannot consciously identify, although
there is no evidence that these “subliminal” stimuli are actually having
an effect on behavior (see Chapter 8).
(39)
Hill (1992) and Iaccino (1998) provide traditional Jungian
analyses of a range of movies while Singh (2009) explores “post-Jungian”
approaches to film criticism.
(40)
The best summary of Jungian theory is the succinct overview
he wrote just prior to his death, Man and His
Symbols (1964).
(41)
Jung’s theory has often been accused of being mystical. One
of Jung’s (1969: pp. 43-44) most compelling responses to this criticism
is an analogy he makes to instincts. He points out that the existence of
inborn instincts—simple patterns of
behavior that are not learned but crucial to survival
(e.g., the rooting reflex in which newborns turn their heads and suck
when their cheeks are lightly stroked)—are not controversial. He claims
that the archetypes are merely patterns of
thought that give people templates for making sense out
of a complicated world.
(42)
Star Wars is a clear
example of Jungian theory because George Lucas was explicitly inspired
by the prominent mythologist Joseph Campbell (1968), whose approach to
mythology is grounded in Jungian theory. Lucas says, “It was very eerie
because in reading The Hero with a Thousand
Faces I began to realize that my first draft of Star Wars was following classic motifs … so
I modified my next draft according to what I’d been learning about
classical motifs and made it a little bit more consistent” (Larsen and
Larsen, 2002: p. 541).
(43)
Iaccino (1998).
(44)
Hill (1992).
(45)
Ricoeur (1974: p. 99).
(46)
Ray (1985: p. 14), quoting Althusser
(1977).
(47)
Many of the critical topics related to cultural psychology
are reviewed in Cole (1996).
(48)
The ideological approach of Louis Althusser had a
significant impact on the foundation of cultural studies. Storey (2009)
provides an introduction to cultural studies while Ryan (2008) has
edited a comprehensive anthology of significant contributions to the
field.
(49)
See Fiske (1989) for a prominent
example.
(50)
Haskell (1973: pp. 327-328).
(51)
Ray (1985: p. 57).
(52)
See Andrew (1976) and Casetti (1999) for reviews of the
history of film theory.
(53)
Metz integrated his previous work on semiotics, Film Language (1974), with psychoanalysis
in the highly influential, The Imaginary
Signifier (1982). Other psychoanalytic interpretations in
film studies are compiled by Kaplan (1990). More recent approaches to
Lacanian interpretation can be found in McGowan and Kunkle
(2004).
(54)
Greenberg (1993: p. 5), a practicing psychoanalyst,
recounts a humbling experience he had at a conference where his
traditional psychoanalytic reading of a film got a cool reception from
the film scholars in the audience. In contrast, great excitement was
created by a hyperclose Lacanian reading of 20 seconds of a Marlene
Dietrich movie that “discovered” sexual hostility in an edit between
seemingly unrelated scenes. In the first scene, a gun was fired off
screen; if one followed the hypothetical trajectory of the bullet into
the next scene (in a completely separate space), it would presumably hit
a male character squarely in the crotch.
(55)
This difficulty may be intentional. Depending on who you
ask, the complexity is either a reflection of the unstable nature of
knowledge, or it is a neo-elitist tactic designed to aggravate
concrete-minded Philistines (e.g., most
Americans).
(56)
Postmodern philosophy and criticism are associated with
Jacques Derrida, Michael Foucault, Richard Rorty, and many
others.
(57)
Summarized from Casetti’s (1999) analysis
of Metz’s concepts of identification, voyeurism, and
fetishism.
(58)
Silverman (1986).
(59)
Mulvey (1986).
(60)
The phallocentric nature of mainstream film parallels
Haskell’s argument except that Mulvey (1986) is more concerned with the
mechanisms of film, not just the
content of
film.
(61)
Other authors such as Modleski (1988) argue that Hitchcock
films intentionally create discomfort in the audience by
self-consciously manipulating the power differentials Mulvey discusses.
The significance of Vertigo to
Hitchcock himself is discussed in Chapter 4.
(62)
The “text only” approach to criticism is associated with
the New Criticism. Classic essays have recently been anthologized by
Davis (2008).
(63)
The implied viewer is a spin on the implied reader, a term
coined by Iser (1974).
(64)
The danger of too many possible criticisms has been made
within film studies itself. After analyzing the process of film
interpretation at its most abstract, Bordwell (1989a) expresses a
weariness for the seemingly endless interpretations that litter his
field. He demonstrates this point by juxtaposing seven separate critical
interpretations of Psycho, arguing
that while all are reasonable, the benefit of having this many different
readings lying around is unclear.
(65)
The tension between the desire for absolute interpretations
of movies and relativistic “eye of the beholder” approaches are in sharp
relief on internet discussion boards like those on IMDB.com. Many
criticisms are expressed that take the form “This film sucks and anybody
who thinks otherwise is an idiot”; these criticisms are then inevitably
followed by pleas for tolerance because “everyone is entitled to his or
her opinion.”
(66)
Bruner was a major figure in the “cognitive revolution” in
the 1960s in which strict behaviorism was supplanted by cognitive
approaches that allowed for the exploration of mental concepts like
memory and imagination. Later in his career, Bruner found that cognitive
psychology had become narrower and more constricted than he intended,
and he wrote two influential books, Actual
Minds, Possible Worlds (1986) and Acts of Meaning (1990), which argue for a merging of
methodologies between the humanities and the social
sciences.
(67)
Bruner (1990: p. 2).
(68)
Bruner (1986: p. 13).
الفصل الثالث: علم الأمراض النفسية، والعلاج النفسي، وفيلم «سايكو»: علماء النفس ومرضاهم في الأفلام
(1)
Camp, et al.
(2010).
(2)
Fleming and Manvell (1985), a psychologist and a film
historian, offer a thematic analysis of representations of insanity.
Zimmerman (2003) takes a literary perspective to demonstrate the
relative sensitivity of certain films. Robinson (2003) and Wedding,
Boyd, and Niemiec (2010) use formal diagnostic criteria argue that some
films are useful in teaching students about mental illness (discussed
further in Chapter 9). Some authors have focused on mental illness in
films for children, especially Disney films (Wahl, et al., 2003; and Lawson and Fouts, 2004).
All contain extensive lists of films depicting mental
illness.
(3)
I use “psychological disorder” as an approximate synonym
for “psychiatric disorders,” “behavior disorders,” “abnormal
psychology,” “mental illness,” and “psychopathology.” Terms like “mad,”
“crazy” or “loony” are more informal, dramatic and pejorative. In one
way or another, they all suggest problems in behavior and thinking that
prevent people from functioning to their fullest
capacity.
(4)
Camp, et al. (2010; p.
148).
(5)
The image does call to mind serial killer, John Wayne Gacy,
but Gacy acted alone and was not the head of a crime
syndicate.
(6)
Fifty years after its release, Psycho holds upmoderately well as a thriller, but it was
when I realized Hitchcock thought of it as a comedy (Truffaut, 1985: pp.
200–202) that I realized its true genius.
(7)
DID is an official diagnosis in current psychiatric
nomenclature (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). However, it is
controversial, and some professionals do not believe it exists in the
extreme form of people developing distinct
“personalities.”
(8)
Believing that an inanimate object is possessed with
sentience is a delusion while seeing a dead body talk is a
hallucination. Both are common symptoms of schizophrenia, not DID.
Students often confuse DID (which is very rare) with schizophrenia
(which is common). Psycho is at least partially to blame for this
confusion.
(9)
The film is based on the book Psycho, by Robert Bloch (1989). Bloch based his story on
the serial killer, Ed Gein, who lived on an isolated farm in Wisconsin
in the 1950s where he killed and dismembered at least 10 women. This
case is also the inspiration for the horror classics The Texas Chainsaw Massacre and The Silence of the
Lambs.
(10)
As documented in Rebello’s fascinatingly detailed,
Alfred Hitchcock and the Making of
Psycho (1990).
(11)
Truffaut (1985: p. 269).
(12)
Hyler, Gabbard, and Schneider (1991). A few recent examples
have been added.
(13)
While still used in everyday language,
nymphomania has not been a formal diagnostic category
for decades.
(14)
Some filmmakers have actually hired professional
consultants to insure that their depictions are realistic—e.g., the
psychological aspects of imprisonment and brutality in Midnight Express (Farber and Green,
1993).
(15)
Additional examples of accurate portrayals are provided in
Robinson (2009).
(16)
The DSM-IV-TR (Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text
Revision, American Psychiatric Association, 2000) is the
diagnostic reference used by most mental health providers in the US. It
contains 16 major diagnostic classes, but a system of subclassification
can lead to hundreds of distinct diagnoses. The major classifications
that are most common in cinematic depictions include psychotic disorders
(schizophrenia), mood disorders (depression and bipolar), anxiety
disorders (post-traumatic stress disorder), personality disorders
(narcissism and paranoia), dissociative disorders (dissociative identity
disorder), and substance-related disorders.
(17)
The primary criticism is that the film suggests that Nash
managed his symptoms without medication which is not consistent with the
account in the biography, A Beautiful
Mind (Nasar, 2001).
(18)
Greenberg (2003) notes that visual hallucinations (Nash’s
imaginary roommate) are relatively rare when compared to auditory
hallucinations (hearing voices), but of course, movies always prefer to
show things.
(19)
See also Brandell (2004), Rabkin (1998), and Walker
(1993).
(20)
Rabkin (1998) offers detailed information about thousands
of psych related movies.
(21)
I use psychotherapy and counseling as synonyms, and I use
psychologist or mental health professional as shorthand for anybody who
works with people to solve interpersonal and emotional problems. There
are differences between forms of treatment (psychoanalysis versus
psychotherapy) and disciplines (psychology versus psychiatry) although
many people are confused by these differences. In part, the inconsistent
and inaccurate use of these terms in the movies is responsible for the
confusion.
(22)
There are exceptions. Psycho was released at heart of the Golden Age yet its
depiction of Dr Richman is a parody of psychiatric mumbo
jumbo.
(23)
In addition to Drs Dippy, Evil and Wonderful described by
Schneider (1987), other categories have been advanced by Orchowski,
Spickard, and McNamara (2006) and Winick (1978).
(24)
Orchowski, Spickard, and McNamara
(2006).
(25)
Pirkis, et al.
(2006).
(26)
Schultz (2005).
(27)
Martin (2007).
(28)
Gabbard (2001) and Schultz (2005).
(29)
Bischoff and Reiter (1999) and Dine Young, et al. (2008).
(30)
Dr Melfi from the Sopranos was another prominent female therapist that
struggled with her attraction to her mobster client although she
uncharacteristically resisted temptation.
(31)
Some passages in the section have been taken verbatim from
Dine Young, et al.
(2008).
(32)
Perlin (1996).
(33)
Pope and Vasquez
(1998).
(34)
Edelson (1993: p.
311).
(35)
Lambert and Bergin (1994).
(36)
Gabbard (2001).
(37)
McDonald and Walter (2009) document the almost universally
negative portrayal of ECT despite the fact that modern techniques have
minimal side effects and have been shown to be an effective treatment
for some cases of severe depression.
(38)
Movies and television make it appear that psychological
profilers are a substantial professional group when in actuality, there
are very few outside of the FBI. I have disillusioned numerous entering
college students with this unfortunate fact.
(39)
The impact of movies on viewers in general is the subject
of Chapters 8 and 9.
(40)
Jorm (2000).
(41)
Wahl (1995).
(42)
Kondo (2008: pp. 250-251).
(43)
Pirkis, et al.
(2006).
(44)
Granello, et al.
(1999).
(45)
Philo (1996).
(46)
Domino (1983).
(47)
Fleming and Manvell (1985: p. 17).
(48)
Sullivan (1953: p. 32). I always start my class in abnormal
psychology with this quote in order to diminish the tendency of students
to approach the topic in a “disorder of the week”
manner.
(49)
Gender is a good example of similarity getting lost in
diversity. While there are some notable differences between men and
women, across many psychological dimensions the genders are very similar
(Hyde, 2005). These similarities are often overshadowed in such popular
tomes as Gray’s mega-selling: Men are from Mars,
Women are from Venus.
(50)
Werner (1980). I use the term here in a broad way that
simply suggests that two domains of human action are similar in at least
one important dimension (although in other ways they may be
substantially different).
(51)
Sleek (1998).
(52)
Siegel (1999).
(53)
Gabbard (2001); Eber and O’Brien (1982); Ringel
(2004).
(54)
Jamieson, Romer, and Jamieson
(2006).
(55)
Schill, Harsch, and Ritter (1990).
(56)
Dine Young, et al.
(2008).
(57)
Edelson (1993: p. 307).
(58)
Brandell (2004).
(59)
Stein (2003).
(60)
The use of film to teach psychology and pass along life
lessons is further explored in Chapter 9.
الفصل الرابع: العبقري المجنون: التكوين النفسي لصنَّاع الأفلام
(1)
Corliss (1992).
(2)
Lax (2000: p. 397).
(3)
Bjorkman (1994).
(4)
See Schultz (2005) and Elms (1994) for overviews of
psychobiography. Both authors point out that as psychology has
established itself as an experimental science, the method has been
marginalized. Lives are too big to fit into laboratories (even the ones
at large universities). Early psychologists like William James thought
that the study of lives could exist side by side with experimental
approaches. Gordon Allport (1965) advocated case studies as a way to
balance statistical generalization. He observed that it might be useful
for a man to have a general understanding of what most women like when
shopping for his wife, but that he was better off knowing his wife’s
personal preferences (p. 159).
(5)
Erikson (1962).
(6)
See Schultz (2005) for chapters on these and other
artists.
(7)
Freud (1957).
(8)
Examples of projective tests include the Thematic
Apperception Test (test-takers tell stories in response to a picture)
and the Kinetic Family Drawing Test (test-takers draw their families in
action).
(9)
There are a few exceptions such as a biography of Charlie
Chaplin written by a psychiatrist (Weissman,
2008).
(10)
Wollen (1976).
(11)
While many directors (Scorsese, Tarantino, Polanski,
etc.) have adopted the Hitchcockian trick of popping up in their own
movies, these appearances don’t generate as much
excitement.
(12)
Spoto (1983: p. x).
(13)
Spoto (1983: p. 9). There is some question whether the
event actually happened. Spoto declares that he was unable to find
evidence to confirm it or refute it (p. 16).
(14)
Spoto (1983: p. 36).
(15)
Spoto (1983: p. 37).
(16)
Spoto (1983: p. 65).
(17)
Spoto (1983: p. 343).
(18)
Spoto (1983: p. 387).
(19)
LoBrutto (2008: p. 32).
(20)
Keyser (1992: p. 7).
(21)
LoBrutto (2008: p. 33).
(22)
Quoted in Keyser (1992: p. 10).
(23)
Cohen-Shalev and Raz (2008).
(24)
Cohen-Shalev and Raz (2008: p.
36).
(25)
Dyer (1998: p. 43).
(26)
McGilligan (1994: pp. 42–47).
(27)
McGilligan (1994: pp. 262–264).
(28)
McGilligan (1994: p. 263).
(29)
McGilligan (1994: pp. 51-52).
(30)
Morton (2010: p. 105).
(31)
Morton (2010: pp. 108-109) attributes these quotes to
Franziska De George and Iris Martin respectively but does not
provide a context for how these professional opinions were acquired,
opening up the possibility they were interpreted out of
context.
(32)
This saying is associated with Gestalt psychology, a
school of psychology that focused on sensation and
perception.
(33)
Bertolucci, Shaw, and Mawson (2003: p.
20).
(34)
Bertolucci, Shaw, and Mawson (2003: p.
25).
(35)
Quoted in Bertolucci, Shaw, and Mawson (2003: p.
28).
(36)
D’Arminio (2011).
(37)
Recent developments in method acting are summarized in
Krasner (2000). Method acting has made the psychobiography of actors
easier since such actors self-consciously access aspects of themselves
in playing their roles.
(38)
Indick (2004).
(39)
Farber and Green (1993: p. 21).
(40)
Farber and Green (1993: p. 80).
(41)
Farber and Green (1993: p. 311).
(42)
For me, Allen’s presentation of psychotherapy in his movies
seemed at once cautionary and intriguing. Growing up in a rural town, I
watched Annie Hall and Manhattan
repeatedly on cable. Woody’s New York seemed an alternative universe in
which people spent their days browsing bookstores, pursuing romance, and
going to therapy. As an undergraduate, I went to New York to interview
for graduate school with a genuine psychoanalyst. I waited while he
argued with his secretary about whether I actually had an appointment.
After an awkward interview during which he glared at me, I made my way
downstairs to find that my car had been towed. I wasn’t accepted by the
school, and my Woody wannabe days were over.
(43)
Lax (2000: p. 79) and Baxter (1999: p.
73).
(44)
Farber and Green (1993: p. 192).
(45)
Cohen (2004) and Philaretou (2006).
(46)
Schultz (2005).
(47)
Based on these criteria, none of the biographical sketches
presented here should be considered definitive. My examples are meant to
capture certain tendencies in psychobiography but are not complete and
accurate pictures of the complicated lives of the filmmakers. The full
biographies I draw upon for my summaries are more detailed, yet it is an
open question as to whether they are accurate, coherent and
consistent.
(48)
Spoto (1983: p. 36).
(49)
Elms (2005).
(50)
Schultz (2005: p. 10).
(51)
Seligman and Csikzentmihalyi (2000).
(52)
Nettle (2001).
(53)
Rothenberg (1990: p. 6).
(54)
Nettle (2001: p. 145).
(55)
Freud (1959) emphasized the unconscious desires of
storytellers, but filmmakers often seem aware of the personal
significance of their movies. Consciousness and unconsciousness are not
discrete states but exist on a continuum and are therefore a matter of
degree.
الفصل الخامس: الجمهور: الأنماط السيكولوجية لروَّاد دُور العَرْض السينمائية
(1)
Zillmann and Bryant (1985).
(2)
Fuller (1996).
(3)
Austin (1989: pp. 35-36). These attendance figures don’t
necessarily refer to the number of different individuals who attend a
movie since some may have attended more than one movie a
week.
(4)
Television Facts and Statistics
(n.d.).
(5)
Austin (1989: p. 36).
(6)
Austin (1989: p. 40).
(7)
Austin (1989: pp. 87–92).
(8)
Taylor (2002).
(9)
Krugman and Johnson (1991).
(10)
Yearly Box Office (2011).
(11)
On an individual level, psychologists occasionally use
reading or viewing tastes as a measure of personality. It seems likely
that someone who watches only horror movies will be different from
someone else who chooses to watch only romantic comedies. This chapter
focuses on general trends in movie viewing. A closer look at movie
enjoyment is considered in later chapters, particularly Chapter
7.
(12)
Lists of box office champs as well as other movies lists
are available at www.filmsite.org. The list I am using
has been adjusted for inflation and is therefore historically balanced.
Movie admissions in 1939 when Gone with the
Wind was released were much cheaper than a 3D showing of
Avatar in 2009. This explains why Avatar, though the highest grossing movie of all time,
is ranked below blockbusters from different economic eras such as
Titanic, The Sound of Music, and
Gone with the
Wind.
(13)
To the extent that I am a representative movie fan, I have
seen 45 of the 50 films and am generally familiar with every movie on
the list except The
Robe.
(14)
Dean Simonton (2011) has compiled a large database of box
office performance, awards, critics’ ratings and other publicly
available information.
(15)
Simonton (2011: pp. 53–78).
(16)
Simonton (2011: p. 82).
(17)
Simonton (2011: p. 102).
(18)
McIntosh, et al.
(2003).
(19)
Simonton (2011).
(20)
Roberts and Foehr (2004).
(21)
Worth et al.
(2008).
(22)
The effects of film are discussed in detail in Chapter
8.
(23)
See Pritzker (2009) for example.
(24)
Marich (2005).
(25)
Retrieved from the American Film Institute’s website at
www.afi.com/100years/movies10.aspx. AFI is an
association of filmmakers, producers and critics who, according to their
website, are dedicated to film preservation and educational
activities.
(26)
Accessed from the Internet Movie Database at
www.imdb.com/chart/top on April 1,
2011.
(27)
Another difference is the handful of foreign films (e.g.,
Seven Samurai) on the IMDB list;
AFI only ranks American movies.
(28)
Compared to the exclusive selection process for members of
the American Film Institute, IMDB allows access to anyone who is online.
Still, there is a strong element of self-selection, as individuals must
choose not only to use the site but also its rating
function.
(29)
Fischoff, et al.
(2002-2003).
(30)
This survey was conducted in the early 2000s before the
Twilight craze. Therefore, the
results aren’t confounded by this massively successful book/movie
series. However, its findings may have anticipated the neovampire craze
of the new millennium.
(31)
Banerjee, et al.
(2008).
(32)
Lincoln and Allen (2004).
(33)
Thinking about movie viewing in terms of “before, during,
and after” establishes a cyclical process. If someone goes to a movie
and has an experience which they evaluate as positive, they will likely
develop a preference for a particular genre or actor and seek to
reproduce it in subsequent movie choices.
الفصل السادس: اللحظة السينمائية: المشاعر واستيعاب الأفلام
(1)
This figure is a variation of Figure 1-3 inspired by Werner
and Kaplan (1984). The symbol is the film, and the “referent” is divided
up into multiple of levels of “images and sound” and “story.” It is
important that the arrows go both ways. My example starts with
perceptual details and move toward the overarching story; this approach
has been described as “bottom-up” processing. However, viewers come to
films with expectations about how stories work that impact the
perceptual elements they pay attention to; this is “top-down”
processing. Humans appear to engage in both types of processing
simultaneously.
(2)
Classical Hollywood style is surveyed in Bordwell, Staiger,
and Thompson (1985).
(3)
Cognitive psychology is an important sub-discipline of
psychology and is summarized in numerous textbooks such as Sternberg and
Sternberg (2011). Cognitive science is an interdisciplinary field that
includes psychology, biology, computer science and philosophy.
Neuroscience focuses on how the functioning of the brain and the rest of
the nervous system impacts thinking and behavior.
(4)
Bordwell (1985; 1989a; 1989b), heavily influenced by early
psychological studies of film by Munsterberg (1970) and Arnheim (1957),
has written several seminal texts outlining a cognitive approach to
narrative comprehension of film. Turner (1996) has made a similar case
in regard to literature. Other early proponents of the cognitive turn in
film studies include Noel Carroll (1988) and Edward Branigan
(1992).
(5)
Grodal (1997), Tan (1996) and Plantinga (2009) are example
of cognitive-based theories of film comprehension and emotion. Hogan
(2003) provides an accessible overview of cognitive approaches in
literature, film and art. Bordwell and Carroll (1996), and Plantinga and
Smith (1999) compile a variety of essays on film that take cognitivism
(as opposed to Lacanian interpretation) as their starting
point.
(6)
See Anderson (1998) and Hochberg (1989) for overviews of
film perception.
(7)
Anderson (1998: pp. 54–61).
(8)
Anderson (1998: pp. 99–101).
(9)
Hochberg (1989).
(10)
While most scholars agree that there is an interaction
between cultural influence and innate endowment, the relative
contribution of these factors remains controversial in all areas of the
social sciences.
(11)
Bordwell (1985).
(12)
Chatman (1978) makes a similar distinction between story
and discourse, while Bordwell (1985) borrows terms from Russian literary
theory: fabula (story) and syuzhet (plot).
(13)
Bordwell (1989a: p. 49).
(14)
The way people are able to take the concept of “dog” and
apply it to a variety of objects in the world is an example of a simple
linguistic schema. A physicist’s understanding of atomic structure is a
more complicated schema.
(15)
Hogan (2003).
(16)
The state of modern emotion research is surveyed in Lewis,
Haviland-Jones, and Barrett’s (2008) edited
volume.
(17)
Grodal (1997) and Plantinga (2009).
(18)
Tan (1996).
(19)
Mauss, et al. (2005)
reported that behavioral, self-report, and physiological responses
tend to be modestly correlated, supporting a connection between
body, consciousness, and behavior.
(20)
See Mauss, et al.
(2005) and Hoffner and Cantor (1991) for examples using adults and
children respectively.
(21)
Mauss, et al.
(2005).
(22)
Tomarken, Davidson, and Henriques
(1990).
(23)
Hubert and de Jong-Meyer (1991).
(24)
Laan, et al. (1994)
and Koukounas and Over (1997). Methods that involve contact between
genitalia and laboratory equipment typically provoke giggles from my
students, and indeed they are among the most intrusive social
science methods. However, for ethical purposes, studies of human
sexuality usually involve thorough education of participants before
they make an informed decision to
participate.
(25)
Rottenberg, Ray, and Gross
(2007).
(26)
Yes, there are even hardened souls out there who would
mock The Champ or laugh at Silence of the
Lambs.
(27)
Holland (1989). There is some question as to whether
the audience exposure part of the Kuleshov experiment was ever
actually conducted or whether Pudovkin extrapolated from his own
introspective observations. The experiment has been a reference
point in film theory and provides a simple example of how an
experiment in editing can be accomplished. I predict that Pudovkin
and Kuleshov’s hypothesis would stand up
today.
(28)
This observation is consistent with Lacanian
psychoanalysis’ claims that spectators “suture” juxtaposed scenes
together.
(29)
Kraft (1991).
(30)
This pattern is often summarized in introductory film
text books such as Barsam and Monahan (2010).
(31)
Aristotle (1967).
(32)
Schank and Abelson (1977).
(33)
Pouliot and Cowen (2007).
(34)
Wollen (1976).
(35)
Summarized from Carroll (1999: pp.
35–46).
(36)
This idea is closely related to Jung’s archetypes
discussed in Ch. 2 although the two concepts emerged from different
theoretical traditions.
(37)
Hogan (2003).
(38)
Identification is a crucial issue in film theory,
psychology and psychoanalysis. Similar terms include “involvement,”
“engagement,” and “participation.” Grodal (1997) reviews some of the
important variations of identification as used in film studies.
While most of these subtleties are not relevant here, the nature of
identification (in regard to type, intensity, and duration) can
impact the effect of film on viewers discussed in Chapters 8 and
9.
(39)
Hoffner (1995).
(40)
Summarized from Plantinga’s (1999) application of Paul
Ekman’s (2007) theory of universal facial
expressions.
(41)
Anderson, et al. (2006:
p. 7).
(42)
Summarized from Hogan (2003: pp.
174–179).
(43)
Major theories of psychological interpretation are
overviewed in Chapter 2.
(44)
Bordwell (1989a).
(45)
The tendency to isolate discrete processes is comparable to
the tendency of medicine to divide up the body in various
subsystems.
(46)
Hogan (2003: p. 3).
الفصل السابع: تأمُّل الشاشة: تلقِّي الأفلام
(1)
Ebert (1986: pp. 173-174).
(2)
Kael (1976: pp. 247–251).
(3)
Thank you, Mom and Dad.
(4)
I didn’t take the film entirely seriously. At a high school debate camp held in
Georgetown, we amused ourselves by holding races up and down the infamous
Exorcist
stairs.
(5)
Some student reactions hardly reflect the film’s reputation:
“How could people ever have thought that was scary?,” “I laughed during the
exorcism scenes,” and “That’s nothing compared to Hostel.”
(6)
Kenneth Burke (1984) has noted that all living things are
critics, using a trout’s dilemma of whether or not to take the bait as a
metaphor for the people, places and things that we either pursue or
avoid.
(7)
Viewer preferences for types are considered in Chapter
5.
(8)
The psychology of entertainment is surveyed in edited
volumes by Bryant and Vorderer (2006) and Zillmann and Vorderer
(2000).
(9)
See discussion of emotions and comprehension in Chapter
6.
(10)
Freud (1960a).
(11)
Zillmann (2000).
(12)
Zillmann (2000).
(13)
Critics argue that many modern action movies are indeed
random expressions of violence. Yet not all movies with explosions
and gunfire are successful, leading to the probability that even
action films draw something from character and
plot.
(14)
Dispositional theory is reviewed in Zillmann
(2011).
(15)
Zillmann (2006).
(16)
See Weaver and Tamborini (1996) for overview of
research on horror.
(17)
Tamborini and Stiff (1987).
(18)
The Alien series has
a third and fourth installment, but they weren’t financially or
critically successful. The fact that they do not end well is consistent with my
point.
(19)
The tendency of the virgin female characters to survive
while sexually active female characters are killed is discussed in
Chapter 2.
(20)
Oliver (1993).
(21)
Oliver (2008).
(22)
Oliver and Woolley (2011).
(23)
It is possible to see evaluation as a subtype of
interpretation since judging a movie as enjoyable can be viewed as a
form of meaning.
(24)
See overview of theoretical approaches to interpretation in
Chapter 2.
(25)
Historical approaches have been used to look at the
reception of many art and narrative forms. For example, Freedberg
(1989) surveys the intense, visceral, and sometimes violent
reactions that audiences have had to public displays of artworks
(both low and high) over the centuries.
(26)
Mayne (1993: p. 148).
(27)
Staiger (2000: p. 162).
(28)
Gina Fournier (2007) offers an exhaustive historical
look at the reception of a single film.
(29)
Fournier (2007: p. 31).
(30)
White and Robinson (1991: p.
29).
(31)
See overview of ideological approaches to film
interpretation in Chapter 2.
(32)
Television has been given more attention than film in
cultural studies. This is a reflection of the Marxist roots of the
field since television viewing provides a more pervasive immersion
in ideological messages than film viewing.
(33)
Morley (1980).
(34)
Ang (1985) and Liebes and Katz (1990) cover the
reception of Dallas. The latter
is the focus of my summary.
(35)
Reader response criticism has been advocated by many
literary scholars such as Iser (1974), Bleich (1978) and Holland
(1989). Tompkin’s (1980) is a compilation of essays by key
figures.
(36)
Holland (1986).
(37)
Young (1992).
(38)
Hill (1999).
(39)
Zillmann (2011).
(40)
Shaw (2004: p. 140-141).
(41)
Film scholars Mayne (1993) and Staiger (1992) explore
various approaches to film spectatorship and reception, while edited
volumes by Bryant and Vorderer (2006), Bryant and Zillmann’s (1991) and
Zillmann and Vorderer (2000) survey social science approaches to media
reception and enjoyment.
(42)
Oliver and Woolley (2011).
(43)
Spectatorship approaches are overviewed in Chapter
2.
(44)
Quotes from the introduction to the first edition of
Reading the Romance (pp. 3-4).
They were removed from the 1991 second edition, because the author felt
that the juxtaposition simplified the positions of the other scholars.
While this may be true, putting such divergent quotes side by side is a
useful rhetorical technique for highlighting the difference between
textual interpretation and the lived experience of
fans.
(45)
Bordwell (1989a).
الفصل الثامن: الأفلام دافع للسلوك: تأثيرات الفيلم
(1)
Block (2007).
(2)
Effects tradition has been subject to many overviews like
Sparks (2010) an undergraduate text. Other texts such as Giles (2003) and
Harris (1999) provide concise summaries of a variety of subdomains. Perse
(2001) is a more advanced overview. Bryant and Oliver (2003) and Nabi and
Oliver (2009) are edited volumes of contributions by many scholars in the
field.
(3)
Ultimately the line between consciousness and nonconsciousness
is not clear-cut. It is not an all-or-nothing phenomenon. Sometimes we are
partially aware of things, other times we are aware of things and then
forget them. Consciousness varies across time and is best understood as a
continuum. The type of impact of a film will vary depending on factors such
as when it was viewed, the conditions of viewing and recall,
etc.
(4)
Blumer (1933).
(5)
Blumer and Hauser (1933).
(6)
Overviewed in Giles (2003).
(7)
See Sparks (2010) and Bryant and Zillman (2009) for historical
summaries of effects research.
(8)
Key (1973).
(9)
Sparks (2010).
(10)
Perloff (2009).
(11)
These topics have been the frequent objects of content
analysis discussed in Chapter 2.
(12)
The Snopes website for debunking media myths claims
this widely reported incident has never been verified. Certain
claims—T-shirt sales dropped 75% after the movie—are improbable
based on the fact that even a successful film is only seen by a
relatively small proportion of the
population.
(13)
Hinds (1993).
(14)
Wilson and Hunter (1983).
(15)
Sparks (2010).
(16)
This is an example of the nonfiction media pointing a
finger at its fictional counterpart.
(17)
Surette (2002).
(18)
Wilson and Hunter (1983).
(19)
Movie imagery even influenced how I see Harris and
Klebold. I can’t separate the image of them stalking the halls
wearing flowing overcoats and holding high powered weapons from
those of The Matrix and The Basketball
Diaries.
(20)
See Kirsh (2006) for an overview, and Gentile (2003)
for an edited overview of media and violence with a special focus on
children.
(21)
One can safely conclude there is “a lot” of violence in
the media, but for amore subtle look at the amount and type of
violence, see Kirsh (2006).
(22)
Recent overviews of media and children include Singer
and Singer (2001) and Strasburger and Wilson
(2002).
(23)
Bandura, Ross, and Ross (1963).
(24)
In a fourth condition a film is shown that features an
adult dressed like a cartoon cat; the aggressive behavior is staged
using unrealistic props.
(25)
Eron (1963).
(26)
Huesmann and Eron (1986).
(27)
“Correlation does not imply causation” is a mantra
taught in all social science research courses. While it’s probably
true that the more years of education one has, the more Woody Allen
movies one has seen, it may not be the case that watching Woody
Allen movies makes one smarter.
(28)
This distinction between “cause” and “contribution” is
used by Grimes, Anderson and Bergen (2008) to distinguish between
the “causationists,” those researchers who take a strong position
that media violence alone causes negative behavioral effects, and
the “contributionists,” who believe that media violence is one
factor that interacts with many others.
(29)
Roskos-Ewoldsen and Roskos-Ewoldsen
(2009).
(30)
Much of this research is summarized in Harris and
Bartlett (2009) and Gunter (2002).
(31)
Collins et al.
(2004).
(32)
However, in regard to college students, another study
(Wilson and Liedtke, 1984) took a straightforward copycat approach
and surveyed college students about which movies had been a
“significant stimulus” for a sex act. 64% of the males and 39% of
the females indicated that at least one film had inspired them
(including 10, Endless Love, The Blue
Lagoon, Saturday Night Fever, and An Officer and a
Gentlemen).
(33)
Harris and Bartlett (2009).
(34)
Harris and Bartlett (2009).
(35)
Titus-Ernstoff et
al. (2008).
(36)
Hazan, Lipton, and Glantz
(1994).
(37)
Stoolmiller et al. (2010).
(38)
Mathai (1983).
(39)
Ballon and Leszcz (2007).
(40)
Ballon and Leszcz (2007); Bozzuto (1975); Hamilton
(1978); and Tenyi and Csizyne (1993).
(41)
Bozzuto (1975).
(42)
Ballon and Leszcz (2007).
(43)
See Chapter 7.
(44)
Hoekstra, Harris, and Helmick
(1999).
(45)
Harrison and Cantor (1999).
(46)
Johnson (1980).
(47)
Cantor (2009).
(48)
Cantor and Omdahl (1999).
(49)
Sparks and Cantor (1986).
(50)
Cantor, Wilson, and Hoffner
(1986).
(51)
Singer and Singer (2005).
(52)
It is primarily for this reason that, despite my
personal love of movies, my wife and I have chosen to limit screen
exposure with our children while they are
young.
(53)
Smith and Granados (2009).
(54)
Levine and Harrison (2008).
(55)
Mastro (2009).
(56)
Busselle and Crandall (2002).
(57)
Perse (2001).
(58)
Linz, Donnerstein, and Penrod
(1988).
(59)
Jowett and O’Donnell (1992).
(60)
Gerbner et al.
(2002)
(61)
McLuhan (1964).
(62)
Postman wrote his book in the 1980s. Today’s prominence of
the computer screen adds another dimension to his
argument.
(63)
McLuhan, Postman, and other cultural critics typically
don’t refer to surveys or experiments, but they do share concerns about
the negative impact of media on society. If one extrapolates the results
of some effects studies across the culture, similar conclusions may be
reached.
(64)
Strasburger and Wilson (2002).
(65)
Following Bandura, Ross, and Ross (1963), psychological
theory has been overshadowed by a litany of atheoretical findings. This
paucity of theory was documented in a content analysis by Potter and
Riddle (2007). Recent attempts such as Nabi and Oliver’s (2009) edited
volume are designed to give the field more conceptual
weight.
(66)
A variety of perspectives on public policy as it relates to
media and children are discussed in Singer and Singer
(2005).
(67)
Perse (2001: p. ix).
(68)
Sparks, Sparks, and Sparks (2009: p.
273).
(69)
Huesmann and Taylor (2003).
(70)
Freedman (2002: p. ix).
(71)
Trend (2007: p. 3).
(72)
See Huesmann and Taylor (2003: pp. 112, 130, 111) for the
three claims, respectively.
(73)
Grimes, Anderson and Bergen (2008: p.
49).
(74)
In particular, the criticism seems to imply that a study
must be perfectly randomly sampled, that there can be no variation in
participant response, and that the measure for the study must exactly
and completely capture the effect (the “construct”) of interest. Such
studies do not exist in the social sciences.
(75)
I once attended a media effects presentation at a national
communication convention where I asked a question about the experiential
dimension of the participants in the study. I intended it as a friendly
question to move the discussion into a slightly different direction. One
of the researchers, however, took my question as an implied denunciation
of the work and suggested I “go down the hall where the rhetoricians are
doing that kind of thing.”
(76)
Centerwall (1993).
(77)
Trend (2007: p. 1).
(78)
Perse (2001).
(79)
Linz, Donnerstein, and Penrod
(1988).
(80)
For those unfamiliar with this movie, the tagline on
IMDB.com says it all: “Danny Bonaduce and a cast of Playboy playmates
get H.O.T.”
(81)
Directed by Amy Heckerling, written by Cameron Crowe, and
featuring talented young stars including Sean Penn and Jennifer Jason
Leigh.
الفصل التاسع: الأفلام كوسيلة للعيش: وظائف الفيلم
(1)
In Widescreen Dreams: Growing Up Gay at
the Movies, Horrigan shares his experience of Dog Day Afternoon, as well as films like
Hello Dolly!, The Sound of Music, and The Poseidon
Adventure, mixing personal reflection with film commentary.
In explaining his choices, he says, “I focus on these [films] … because they
happened to be the movies that meant the most to me as I was growing up and
because in writing about them, I’m trying to understand as fully as possible
who I am and why I think and feel as I do” (p. xix).
(2)
Horrigan (1999: p. xix).
(3)
Using movies self-reflectively is not inherently a good thing.
A viewer may make life choices based on a film that she subsequently comes
to regret (e.g., “I should never have believed that Prince Charming would
rescue me after seeing Pretty Woman”).
Alternatively, a viewer could be happy with the impact of a film on his life
(“Rambo convinced me that might makes
right”), yet have that impact judged negatively by
others.
(4)
Fisch (2009).
(5)
This is an example of multimedia synchronicity. While
writing this section, I recalled a movie about a wooden Indian in a
boat, but couldn’t remember the title. I Googled the plot and, to my
delight, found it was called Paddle-to-the-Sea. On IMDB.com I learned it was a
short film based on the book of the same name by Holling Clancy
Holling. The next day I happened to be watching the 1990s TV show,
Northern Exposure on DVD. In
the episode, “The Final Frontier,” the erudite disc jockey Chris
(John Corbett) is reading Paddle-to-the-Sea on air. Northern Exposure is a favorite of mine. The episode
“Rosebud,” which uses Citizen
Kane to make the point that movies are modern healing
myths, was first aired at the same time I was reading Kenneth
Burke’s essay “Literature as Equipment for Living.” These influences
shaped my research program and much of this chapter. And taking it
back even further, Northern
Exposure is clearly a version of Sesame Street transplanted to Alaska
with adults and no Muppets.
(6)
Wonderly (2009: p. 12).
(7)
Murray (1979).
(8)
Sutherland and Feltey (2009).
(9)
Van Belle and Mash (2009).
(10)
Murray and Heumann (2009).
(11)
Alexander, Lenahan, and Pavlov
(2005).
(12)
Paddock, Terranova, and Giles
(2001).
(13)
Wedding, Boyd, and Niemic
(2010).
(14)
Dr Fritz Engstrom leads summer workshops at the Cape
Cod Institute where therapists reflect on psychology and film in the
morning and enjoy the beach in the afternoon—the good
life.
(15)
Kerby et al.
(2008).
(16)
Gladstein and Feldstein (1983).
(17)
Cinematherapy was preceded by bibliotherapy, the use of books to promote
therapeutic change (e.g., Pardeck, 1993). The term cinematherapy was first used by
Berg-Cross, Jennings, and Baruch (1990) although the therapeutic use
of film appeared earlier (Smith, 1974). Hesley and Hesley (2001),
Rubin (2008), and Gregerson (2010) are all extensions of
cinematherapy and other uses of popular culture in
counseling.
(18)
Kuriansky et al.
(2010).
(19)
Turley and Derdeyn (1990).
(20)
This is the approach of Hesley and Hesley in their
Rent Two Films and Let’s Talk in the
Morning.
(21)
Shedler (2010).
(22)
Unfortunately Jones (2002) fails to consistently
confront the studies on negative impacts of violence, an example of
how the humanities and the social sciences remain
segregated.
(23)
Madison and Schmidt (2001).
(24)
Grace (2006).
(25)
Niemiec and Wedding (2008).
(26)
Positive psychology encompasses many areas of
psychology including clinical, personality, developmental, social,
and neuropsychology. The movement was popularized by Seligman and
Csikszentmihalyi (2000), building upon Csikszentmihalyi’s (1997)
work on “flow” (those moments when people are at their optimal level
of functioning) and related concepts.
(27)
Peterson and Seligman (2004).
(28)
See Blumler and Katz (1974), Rosengren, Wenner and
Palmgreen (1985), and Rubin (2009) for overviews of uses and
gratifications research.
(29)
Katz, Blumler and Gurevitch (1974: pp. 21-22). Rubin (2009)
points out that recent study has been more interested in practical
implications.
(30)
See Chapter 7 for an overview of this
issue.
(31)
See Zillmann (1988), and Knobloch-Westerick
(2006).
(32)
Note that “media” is embedded in the term “mediated,” a form of communication in
which the text/screen/sound is a symbolic representation of its
creator(s).
(33)
Perse and Rubin (1990).
(34)
Radway’s (1991: p. 61) study is discussed in more detail in
Chapter 7.
(35)
See Chapter 3 for discussion on portrayal of mental health
professionals and mental illness.
(36)
Wright (1974).
(37)
Tesser, Millar, and Wu (1988).
(38)
Oliver and Woolley (2011).
(39)
Burke (1973: p. 304).
(40)
The importance of symbolism runs throughout Burke’s (1966;
1973) writings.
(41)
See Dine Young (1996, 2000) for further discussion of this
phenomenon.
(42)
See Chapters 6 and 7 for further exploration of these
ideas.
(43)
Narrative approaches to knowledge are discussed in Chapter
2.
(44)
McAdams (1993).
(45)
Mar and Oatley (2008: p. 183).
(46)
Mar and Oatley (2008: p. 186).
(47)
Brummett (1985).
(48)
Qualitative audience response methods allow scholars to
consider idiosyncratic experiences that may not be typical. For example,
the notion of catharsis has been widely rejected in the effect tradition
in regard to aggressive (Bandura, 2009) and sexual (Harris and Bartlett,
2009) impulses. Given a broad sample of participants, it is difficult to
systematically demonstrate that most people will experience a deflation
of intense emotions (such as aggression) when exposed to emotional films
(as opposed to assimilating the emotions of the film). This doesn’t mean
that catharsis never happens. Perhaps it is a more subtle, reflective
process that occurs when people with sufficient ego strength are exposed
to a well-done fictional narrative in a safe environment. Could such
exposure help some people modulate aggressive tendencies in everyday
life? Instances supporting this claim would me more accessible in
open-ended interviews than they would be in social psych
experiments.
(49)
See Rubin (1996) for an overview of autobiographical
memory.
(50)
See Fivush and Haden (2003) for an edited volume
exploring the relationship between narratives and autobiographical
memory.
(51)
See section on psychiatric disturbances in Chapter
8.
(52)
Stein (1993).
(53)
McAdams (1993).
(54)
McMillan (1991).
(55)
Dine Young (2000).
(56)
All subjects from my interviews were assigned
pseudonyms to insure confidentiality.
(57)
See Hills (2002) for overview of fan
theory.
(58)
Austin (1981).
(59)
See Lieblich, McAdams, and Josselson (2004) and White
and Epston (1990) as examples of narrative therapy and Payne (1989)
for the therapeutic use of rhetoric.
(60)
Heinz Werner (1980) argues that development is more
than just the aging process. What comes later cannot automatically
be assumed to be more developed than what comes before. Instead,
development is a conceptual framework that assumes that some modes
of functioning have advantages over other modes and can therefore be
said to have “progressed,” become “higher developed” or even to be
“better.”
(61)
Dine Young (1996).
الفصل العاشر: خاتمة: الصورة الكاملة
(1)
For the record, I am not a Star
Wars purist. I don’t mind Lucas tinkering with the special
effects, and I enjoyed Episodes I-III.
Nor I am particularly troubled by the imperious tone Lucas sometimes takes
in interviews, and I am content with his decision to leave the series at six
episodes. However, I was bothered when he started claiming in the 1990s that
he had never intended a third trilogy. This seemed like a violation of the
accepted fact that my pre-adolescent friends and I pondered endlessly, like
a rug being pulled out from under my thirty-something
self.
(2)
This figure is essentially a combination of Figure 1-3 and
Figure 8-3.
(3)
Two other important dimensions of the film experience
(conscious versus non-conscious; social versus individual) that I have
repeatedly emphasized cannot be captured in Figure 10-4 without going
3-D.
(4)
Actually, I do remember that The Goodbye Girl starred
Richard Dreyfuss but that was only because he would soon
end up in a kid-friendly Spielberg movie, Close Encounters of the Third
Kind.
(5)
Filmmakers also engage in multiple levels of psychological
processing as they employ perceptual technologies (cameras), write
scripts, and draw upon themes that resonate in their own lives. More
scholarly attention has been paid to viewers mostly because they are a
larger and more accessible group than filmmakers.
(6)
See Ch. 8 for an overview of these
dangers.